Monday, July 8, 2024

Why Attitude Is More Important Than IQ 



When it comes to success, it’s easy to think that people blessed with brains are inevitably going to leave the rest of us in the dust. But new research from Stanford University will change your mind (and your attitude).

Psychologist Carol Dweck has spent her entire career studying attitude and performance, and her latest study shows that your attitude is a better predictor of your success than your IQ…..Story continues

By: Dr. Travis Bradberry

Source: Why Attitude Is More Important Than IQ – TalentSmartEQ – Emotional Intelligence Training

.

Critics:

The theory of planned behavior suggests that behaviors are primarily influenced by the attitude and other intentions. The theory of planned behavior was proposed by Icek Ajzen in 1985 through his article “From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior.” The theory was developed from the theory of reasoned action, which was proposed by Martin Fishbein together with Icek Ajzen in 1975.

The theory of reasoned action was in turn grounded in various theories of attitude such as learning theories, expectancy-value theories, consistency theories, and attribution theory. According to the theory of reasoned action, if people evaluate the suggested behavior as positive (attitude), and if they think their significant others want them to perform the behavior (subjective norm), this results in a higher intention (motivation) and they are more likely to do so.

A high correlation of attitudes and subjective norms to behavioral intention, and subsequently to behavior, has been confirmed in many studies. The theory of planned behavior contains the same component as the theory of reasoned action, but adds the component of perceived behavioral control to account for barriers outside one’s own control.

Russell H. Fazio proposed an alternative theory called “Motivation and Opportunity as Determinants” or MODE. Fazio believes that because there is deliberative process happening, individuals must be motivated to reflect on their attitudes and subsequent behaviors. Simply put, when an attitude is automatically activated, the individual must be motivated to avoid making an invalid judgement as well as have the opportunity to reflect on their attitude and behavior.

The MODE (motivation and opportunity as determinants of the attitude-behavior relation) model was developed by Fazio. The MODE model, in short is a theory of attitude evaluation that attempts to predict and explain behavioral outcomes of attitudes. When both are present, behavior will be deliberate. When one is absent, impact on behavior will be spontaneous. A person’s attitude can be measured explicitly and implicitly.

The model suggests whether attitude activation occurs and, therefore, whether selective perception occurs depends on attitude accessibility. More accessible attitudes are more likely to be activated in a behavioral situation and, therefore, are more likely to influence perceptions and behavior. A counter-argument against the high relationship between behavioral intention and actual behavior has also been proposed, as the results of some studies show that, because of circumstantial limitations, behavioral intention does not always lead to actual behavior.

Namely, since behavioral intention cannot be the exclusive determinant of behavior where an individual’s control over the behavior is incomplete, Ajzen introduced the theory of planned behavior by adding a new component, “perceived behavioral control.” By this, he extended the theory of reasoned action to cover non-volitional behaviors for predicting behavioral intention and actual behavior.

Another classic view of attitudes is that attitudes serve particular functions for individuals. That is, researchers have tried to understand why individuals hold particular attitudes or why they hold attitudes in general by considering how attitudes affect the individuals who hold them. Daniel Katz, for example, writes that attitudes can serve “instrumental, adjustive or utilitarian,” “ego-defensive,” “value-expressive,” or “knowledge” functions.

This functional attitude theory suggests that in order for attitudes to change (e.g., via persuasion), appeals must be made to the function(s) that a particular attitude serves for the individual. As an example, the ego-defensive function might be used to influence the racially prejudicial attitudes of an individual who sees themselves as open-minded and tolerant. By appealing to that individual’s image of themselves as tolerant and open-minded, it may be possible to change their prejudicial attitudes to be more consistent with their self-concept.

Similarly, a persuasive message that threatens self-image is much more likely to be rejected.Daniel Katz classified attitudes into four different groups based on their functions.

  1. Utilitarian: provides general approach or avoidance tendencies
  2. Knowledge: organizes and interprets new information
  3. Ego-defensive: protects self-esteem
  4. Value-expressive: expresses central values or beliefs

People adopt attitudes that are rewarding and that help them avoid punishment. In other words, any attitude that is adopted in a person’s own self-interest is considered to serve a utilitarian function. For example, a person who has a condo would pay property taxes. If that leads to an attitude that “increases in property taxes are bad”, then the attitude is serving a utilitarian function.

Several studies have shown that knowledge increases are associated with heightened attitudes that influence behavior. The framework for knowledge is based on significant values and general principles. Attitudes achieve this goal by making things fit together and make sense. As a result, people can maintain a sense of stability and meaning within their worldview. For example:

  1. I believe that I am a good person.
  2. I believe that good things happen to good people.
  3. Something bad happens to Bob.
  4. So, I believe Bob must not be a good person.

When a person is relying on a single dimension of knowledge and that dimension is not directly related to their behavior goal, that person might conclude that the attitude is wrong. This function involves psychoanalytic principles where people use defense mechanisms to protect themselves from psychological harm. Mechanisms include denialrepressionprojection, and rationalization.

The ego-defensive notion correlates with Downward Comparison Theory, which argues that derogating a less fortunate other increases a person’s own subjective well-being. A person is more likely to use the ego-defensive function when they suffer a frustration or misfortune. Identity and social approval are established by central values that reveal who we are and what we stand for. Individuals define and interpret situations based on their central values.

An example would be attitudes toward a controversial political issue. Attitudes can be changed through persuasion and an important domain of research on attitude change focuses on responses to communication. Experimental research into the factors that can affect the persuasiveness of a message include:

Target characteristics: These are characteristics that refer to the person who receives and processes a message. One such trait is intelligence – it seems that more intelligent people are less easily persuaded by one-sided messages. Another variable that has been studied in this category is self-esteem.

Although it is sometimes thought that those higher in self-esteem are less easily persuaded, there is some evidence that the relationship between self-esteem and persuasibility is actually curvilinear, with people of moderate self-esteem being more easily persuaded than both those of high and low self-esteem levels.

Source characteristics: The major source characteristics are expertise, trustworthiness and interpersonal attraction or attractiveness. The credibility of a perceived message has been found to be a key variable here; if one reads a report about health and believes it came from a professional medical journal, one may be more easily persuaded than if one believes it is from a popular newspaper.

Some psychologists have debated whether this is a long-lasting effect and Hovland and Weiss found the effect of telling people that a message came from a credible source disappeared after several weeks (the so-called sleeper effect). Whether there is a sleeper effect is controversial. Perceived wisdom is that if people are informed of the source of a message before hearing it, there is less likelihood of a sleeper effect than if they are told a message and then told its source.

Message Characteristics: The nature of the message plays a role in persuasion. Sometimes presenting both sides of a story is useful to help change attitudes. When people are not motivated to process the message, simply the number of arguments presented in a persuasive message will influence attitude change, such that a greater number of arguments will produce greater attitude change.

Bohner, Gerd; Wanke, Michaela (2011). Attitudes and Attitude Change. Frontiers of Social Psychology (1 ed.). London: Taylor and Francis. doi:10.4324/9781315784786ISBN 9781315784786.

^ Albarracín, Dolores, ed. (2021). “Chapter 1 Definitions, Overview, Goals, and Principles of Cognitive Processing”. Action and Inaction in a Social World: Predicting and Changing Attitudes and Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 1–42. ISBN 978-1-108-84000-2. Retrieved 2023-12-09.

^ Aiken, Lewis R. (2002). Attitudes and Related Psychological Constructs: Theories, Assessment, and Research. Thousand Oaks: Sage Thousand Oaks. ISBN 978-0-7619-2452-4.

^ “APA Dictionary of Psychology”. American Psychological Association. 2018-04-19. Retrieved 2023-11-24.

^ Eagly, Alice H.; Chaiken, Shelly (1998). “Attitude Structure and Function”. In Gilbert, Daniel Todd; Fiske, Susan T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). The Handbook of Social Psychology. McGraw-Hill. ISBN 978-0-19-521376-8.

^ Ajzen, Icek (2001). “Nature and Operation of Attitudes”. Annual Review of Psychology52: 27–58. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.27PMID 11148298S2CID 15064083.

Jump up to:a b Vogel, T., Bohner, G., & Wanke, M. (2014). Attitudes and attitude change. Psychology Press.

^ Haddock, Geoffrey; Maio, Gregory R. (2004). Contemporary Perspectives on the Psychology of Attitudes. New York: Psychology Press Inc. ISBN 1-84169-326-X.

^ Sherman, Jeffrey W.; Klein, Samuel A. W. (2021). “The Four Deadly Sins of Implicit Attitude Research”. Frontiers in Psychology. 11: 604340. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.604340ISSN 1664-1078PMC 7849589PMID 33536976.

^ Hitlin, Steven; Pinkston, Kevin (2013). “Introduction: A Sociological Map”. In DeLamater, John; Ward, Amanda (eds.). Handbook of Social Psychology. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. pp. 319–339. ISBN 978-94-007-6771-3.

^ Wood, W. (2000). “Attitude Change: Persuasion and Social Influence”. Annual Review of Psychology51: 539–570. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.539PMID 10751980S2CID 4944989.

^ Marcinkowski, Tom; Reid, Alan (2019-04-03). “Reviews of research on the attitude–behavior relationship and their implications for future environmental education research”. Environmental Education Research. 25 (4): 459–471. doi:10.1080/13504622.2019.1634237ISSN 1350-4622S2CID 198645699.

Jump up to:a b Leandre, Joshua; Fabringar, Guyer (February 2015). “The attitude-behavior link: A review of the history”. International Encyclopedia of Social Behavioral Sciences: 908–913 – via Research Gate.

^ “Implicit vs. Explicit Attitudes: Definition, Examples & Pros/Cons – Video & Lesson Transcript – Study.com”. education-portal.com. Archived from the original on 20 February 2015. Retrieved 8 May 2018.

^ Zimmerman, Ian. “Implicit Attitudes Predict Impulsive Behavior”. Psychology Today. Retrieved 2023-11-28.

^ Muschalik, Carolin; Elfeddali, Iman; Candel, Math J. J. M.; de Vries, Hein (2018-04-25). “A longitudinal study on how implicit attitudes and explicit cognitions synergistically influence physical activity intention and behavior”. BMC Psychology. 6 (1): 18. doi:10.1186/s40359-018-0229-0ISSN 2050-7283PMC 5921561PMID 29699574.

^ DeDreu, 2003[full citation needed]

^ Krosnick, J. A.; Judd, C. M.; Wittenbrink, B. (2005). “The Measurement of Attitudes”. In Albarracín, Delores; Johnson, Blair T.; Zanna, Mark P. (eds.). The Handbook of Attitudes (PDF). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 21–76. ISBN 978-0805844931. Retrieved November 28, 2023.

^ Olson, James M.; Zanna, Mark P. (1993). “Attitudes and Attitude Change”. Annual Review of Psychology44: 117–54. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.44.1.117.

^ Buhrmester, Michael D.; Blanton, Hart; William, B. Swann Jr (2011). “Implicit self-esteem: nature, measurement, and a new way forward”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (2): 365–385. doi:10.1037/a0021341PMID 21038971S2CID 12654510.

^ Whitley, B. E. (2010). The Psychology of Prejudice & Discrimination. United States: Wadsworth Engage Learning.

^ Fazio, Russell H.; Olson, Michael A. (2003). “Implicit Measures in Social Cognition Research: Their Meaning and Use” (PDF). Annual Review of Psychology. Annual Reviews. 54 (1): 297–327. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225ISSN 0066-4308PMID 12172003S2CID 8797951. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-03-23. Retrieved 2012-04-19.

^ Sekaquaptewa, D., Espinoza, P., Thompson, M., Vargas, P., & von Hippel, W. (2003). Stereotypic explanatory bias: Implicit stereotyping as a predictor of discrimination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 75-82

^ Fabrigar, Leandre R.; MacDonald, Tara K.; Wegener, Duane T. (2005-04-01). “The Structure of Attitudes”. In Albarracín, Dolores; Johnson, Blair T.; Zanna, Mark P. (eds.). The Handbook of Attitudes. Routledge Handbooks Online. doi:10.4324/9781410612823ISBN 978-0-8058-4492-4. Retrieved 2023-12-09.

^ Rosenberg, Milton J.; Hovland, Carl I.; McGuire, William J.; Abelson, Robert P.; Brehm, Jack W. (1960). Attitude Organization and Change: An Analysis of Consistency Among Attitude Components. Yales Studies in Attitude and Communication. Vol. III. Oxford, England: Yale University Press.

^ Eagly, Alice H.; Chaiken, Shelly (1998). “Attitude Structure and Function”. In Gilbert, Daniel Todd; Fiske, Susan T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). In Handbook of Social Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. pp. 269–322. ISBN 978-0-19-521376-8. Retrieved December 12, 2023.

^ Fazio, Russell H., and Michael A. Olson (2003). Attitudes: Foundations, Functions, and Consequences. The Sage Handbook of Social Psychology. London: Sage.

^ Maio, Gregory R.; Haddock, Geoffrey (2009). The Psychology of Attitudes and Attitude Change. SAGE Publications. ISBN 978-1-4129-2975-2. Archived from the original on 2023-04-18. Retrieved 2022-08-04.

^ Breckler, SJ (1984). “Empirical validation of affect, behavior, and cognition as distinct components of attitude”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 47 (6): 1191–1205. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.47.6.1191PMID 6527214.

No comments:

Post a Comment

How To Create High Converting Funnels For Any Marketing Goal Through LaunchPad

Credit to:  arminhamidian The 20+ premium apps included in  Launchpad  are designed to provide comprehensive business launching tools. These...